The University of British Columbia
UBC - A Place of Mind
The University of British Columbia
UBC News
  • Home
  • News Tips
  • Arts & Humanities
  • Business, Law & Society
  • Science, Health & Technology
  • University News
  • Contacts
  • Services
    • Services for Journalists
    • Services for Faculty
Home / 2012 / January / 03 / Look who’s talking! What we are learning from babies about language acquisition
  • Click to share on Twitter (Opens in new window)
  • Click to share on Facebook (Opens in new window)
  • Click to share on LinkedIn (Opens in new window)
  • Click to email this to a friend (Opens in new window)

Look who’s talking! What we are learning from babies about language acquisition

Jan 3, 2012 - by By Janet Werker, Director, UBC Infant Studies Centre, Professor, Department of Psychology

Professor Werker is an organizer and speaker for the AAAS symposium The Effects of Early Experience on Lifelong Functioning: Commitment and Resilience, Friday, Feb. 17, 1:30 – 4:30 p.m.

One of the greatest joys for new parents is to hear their baby utter that first word. “Mama,” “Dada,” or even “juju” (the name of the family dog) are each greeted with enthusiasm and pride. These first spoken words were already preceded by understanding. For example, by six months babies recognize highly frequent words such as their name. But even the achievement of word recognition is not the beginning of language acquisition; it has begun several months earlier, even in the womb.

Language acquisition begins in the womb

Human infants are born with a predisposition to listen to human speech, and to watch the faces of people when they are talking. Although the preparedness for acquiring language is deeply engrained in our biology, it is not ‘language writ large’ that the child has to acquire, but rather the particular language they experience. The perceptual systems are the child’s first entry into acquisition of the native language. They are able to hear native speech beginning at around 26 weeks gestation. By this time the peripheral auditory system has developed, and speech spoken by the mother – and also in the world outside the womb – causes vibrations in the amniotic fluid that displace the eardrum, and hence conduct ‘sound’ to the brain.

Infants do learn about their native language in utero. At birth they show a preference not only for their mother’s voice, but also for the native language. Babies who were exposed to two languages in utero show a preference for both of their native languages. These preferences highlight attention in the neonate to the speech of their native language, and this in turn is thought to help them learn more about the properties of that native language.

Watching talking faces plays a role

Over the next several months of life, infants learn more and more about the properties of the native language. Much of this learning involves attuning, or reorganizing sensitivities that were present at birth. For example, young infants can discriminate many more speech sound differences than they will need in any particular language, and gradually narrow their discrimination sensitivities to just native language sounds. The most famous example here is that young Japanese-learning infants are better able than their parents to respond to the difference between ‘ra’ and ‘la.’ Watching talking faces also plays a role: while a young infant can discriminate French from English just by watching silent talking faces, by the time they are eight months old, infants no longer do so unless they are growing up bilingual. In addition to tuning to the speech sound differences used in their native language, babies also tune to the rhythm and melody of the native language, and learn the sound sequences that occur within words versus those that might signal a boundary between two words.

The perceptual tuning that takes place during the first months of life enable the infant to pull out individual words from the speech stream they hear around them, a skill needed to eventually attach meaning to those words. Thus prior to understanding and speaking that first word, an enormous amount of development has already occurred. By charting when speech-processing changes occur, and how they relate to other abilities that are emerging in the child, we have a far better understanding of how language development unfolds. This positions us to better understand what might happen when there are perturbations in the timing of any particular part of the system, and how to intervene to ensure language development gets back on course.

Find other stories about: AAAS, learning

Young Japanese-learning infants are better able than their parents to respond to the difference between ‘ra’ and ‘la.’

News Tips

News Tips

Looking for story ideas?
Check out our latest News Tips!

Find UBC Experts

An information source that gives journalists access to UBC’s expertise.

Making a Difference

COVID-19

Learn how UBC is rallying to support Canada's response to COVID-19.

Explore

  • Arts & Humanities
  • Business, Law & Society
  • Science, Health & Technology
  • University News
  • Q&As
  • Media Advisories
  • Latest News

Tweets by @ubcnews

My Tweets
Subscribe & receive news by e-mail View UBC's Okanagan News Room
    
Public Affairs
310 - 6251 Cecil Green Park Road
Vancouver, BC Canada V6T 1Z1
Tel 604 822 6397
Fax 604 822 2684
Website http://news.ubc.ca
Email public.affairs@ubc.ca
Find us on
    
Back to top
The University of British Columbia
  • Emergency Procedures |
  • Terms of Use |
  • Copyright |
  • Accessibility
loading Cancel
Post was not sent - check your email addresses!
Email check failed, please try again
Sorry, your blog cannot share posts by email.